The problem with all the philosophical theories of humor that I have read is that they are
one dimensional: They tend to be
either Superiority Theories, or Incongruity
Theories,
or some other single thing.

My contention is that laughter is a very
complex physiological reaction, and in order to
elicit it, many different factors must be present simultaneously: There must be
Structure, Content, and Context. And each of those categories is itself complex.

Under
Structure, the event must have at least one of the following three elements: (1)
Incongruity, (2) Surprise, and (3) Recognition.

(1) By
incongruity I mean that something must be out of place, or inappropriate for a
certain situation.

If a dignified man were making a thank-you speech at an Academy Award ceremony
and suddenly his pants fell down, that would be incongruous. On the other hand, if a
clown at a circus takes a bow and his pants fall down, that would not be particularly
incongruous, because that is a traditional part of a clown act.
    
If a politician is making a serious speech and someone runs on stage and hits him in
the face with a pie, that would be incongruous. But if it were a sketch on
Saturday
Night Live
, it would not be out of place at all.
    
When George W. Bush mispronounces a word, that is incongruous, because the
president of the United States is expected to be an educated person. But if a child
were to mispronounce the same word, it would not be particularly out of place, because
we know a child is still struggling to learn the language.
    
So incongruity, in itself, may or may not elicit a laugh; it depends on other factors.


(2) By
surprise I mean that something happens unexpectedly. One of the funniest
events in TV history happened at an Academy Award ceremony, when a man suddenly
streaked across the stage in the nude. Because it was such a surprising and
inappropriate action, the streaking itself created a huge laugh. But to make the scene
even funnier, David Niven, the master of ceremonies, topped it by saying, “That man
may be the only person in the world who was proud to show off his shortcomings.” That
he was able to think of such a witty response so quickly was even more surprising. And
the word “shortcomings” was also a
double-entente – which was still more surprising.
    
Misdirection is another type of surprise. A comedian starts a joke by leading the
audience to think he is talking about one thing, but by ending the joke with an
unexpected word, we see that he was really talking about something else. Puns and
malapropisms are all examples of words being used in inappropriate and surprising
ways.
    
If you see a funny movie, you will usually not want to see it again, because the gags will
no longer be surprising. If you have heard a joke before, then you won’t laugh at it when
someone tells it again – except perhaps to be polite.
    
If you are broadsided by a drunk driver, that is a surprise. But it probably won’t make
you laugh.
    
So surprise, in itself, may or may not elicit laughter. It depends on other factors.

(3) By
recognition, I mean that the audience must understand the joke. When Jay
Leno makes a joke about some famous person, we must first
recognize the name and
secondly remember why they have recently been in the news; otherwise we won’t “get”
the joke. All “topical” humor requires an audience which reads newspapers and knows
what is going on in the world.
    
When Rich Little, Dana Carvey, or Darrell Hammond do an impression of some
famous person, we must be able to recognize
who they are impersonating or we won’t
think it is funny. This requires first that the audience be
familiar with the person being
imitated, and secondly that the actor be skilled enough to create an accurate
imitation
of their most recognizable characteristics. The fact that a skilled impressionist is able
to transform themselves into some other easily recognizable person is, in itself, both
incongruous and surprising.
    
If you were standing in a theater line and suddenly turn around and find yourself facing
your next door neighbor, you are both almost certain to laugh. Because you (1) instantly
recognize each other, (2) recognize that you are both in an unfamiliar setting, and (3)
such a meeting was totally surprising.
    
Another element of recognition that is frequently overlooked is recognition of a high
degree of
probability. In other words, a gag must be plausible. In the genre of slapstick
farce, such as The Three Stooges, The Marx Brothers, Keystone Kops, Abbot and
Costello, etc., there is plenty of incongruity, the incongruities are frequently unexpected,
and we understand why we are
supposed to laugh. But sophisticated adults usually
don’t laugh – because the situations depicted are so exaggerated as to be
implausible. We recognize that such a thing would not really happen. Children, on the
other hand, find farce much funnier than their parents do because virtually everything in
life is surprising to them. Adults don’t laugh as much as they did when they were
children because they have “been there, done that, and heard that.” Nothing surprises
them anymore. Adults appreciate more sophisticated humor, such as word play
regarding arcane subjects that they haven’t heard before, and that children and
uneducated adults probably would not
understand.
    
Part of the popularity of “Reality” television shows is this element of veracity. When
something happens that we think is unscripted, it is much funnier than it would be on a
Situation Comedy, which we know has been written, acted, and edited. But even with
unscripted TV shows, we know that the program has at least been edited in order to
show us only the funniest moments. So when something incongruous and unexpected
happens in real life, that will strike us as the funniest of all.
    
I think the longest and most raucous laugh I ever heard happened one time during a
speech contest for Atheists United. There was a banner for Atheists United mounted
on the wall behind the lectern. Just as one of the orators reached the climax of his
speech, militantly proclaiming that “There is no God!” suddenly the banner came
crashing to the floor – as though God were sending a message to us evil atheists. If
this event were presented as a skit on
Saturday Night Live, the audience would
probably laugh, but not as hard – because everyone would
recognize that even though
it
could happen, it was only something that someone planned as a joke.
     
As far as
Structure is concerned, at least one of the preceding elements must be
present in order for something to strike us as funny. But the
more elements it has, the
funnier it will be. The example of the speech contest was the funniest of all, because for
the banner to fall at any time during the program would have been incongruous. But for
it to fall perfectly on cue was certainly surprising. And we all knew that it happened
completely by accident. So, all three elements were present in spades.
    
Finally, the recognition element must happen very suddenly. The realization of what the
joke is about must strike the viewer or listener as suddenly as a Karate chop. That’s
why comedians say “It’s all in the timing.” If Jay Leno fluffs a word during a joke, then it
blunts the impact of the point. This suddenness is part of the surprise.

Under the heading of
Content, I mean that the gag (whether acted or spoken) must be
about some subject. Some subjects are inherently ripe for ridicule; some are not.
Those that create a certain level of anxiety in the audience are the most fruitful. These
are the ones which are surrounded by taboos. They are not fit for “polite” conversation.
They are not “politically correct.” Traditionally, they include sex, religion, aggression,
politics, and crime or tragedy. These are things you are
not supposed to joke about.
Breaking these taboos can be out of place (incongruous), unexpected (surprising), and
if the audience knows exactly what the comedian is referring to (recognizable).

Under the heading of
Context, I am referring to the time and place of an event, the
people involved in the event (actors, or other subjects), the composition of the
audience and their expectations.
    
When breaking a taboo, the comic must correctly analyze the total
context of the
situation in order to make a gag “work.” He is always walking on thin ice. A joke must
be close enough to the cutting edge of what is acceptable, but without stepping over
the line. A joke about religion might work very well in a comedy club, but totally bomb at
a church social. On the other hand, it might work very well at a Unitarian church, but not
a Baptist one.
    
Let’s say our atheist speaker had been delivering his speech at the invitation of a
church which had agreed to hear representatives from various religious points of view.
When the banner fell at the climax of his speech, an audience of fundamentalists might
well have gasped in awe at the miracle they had just witnessed – which demonstrated
God’s displeasure at the speech. But it was hilarious to an atheist audience, which
perceived the event as a pure coincidence.

Recently, Jay Leno tried to make a joke about 9/11. It bombed. He said, “Oops... too
soon.” The subject is still too raw to make jokes about it. In a few more months perhaps
the wounds will have healed enough to laugh. And the laughter will help us heal.
    
To joke about a person is to
attack that person. So a comedian must assess whether it
is safe to attack a certain famous person in front of a particular audience. If the person
attacked is perceived as an enemy of the audience, they will laugh uproariously; but if it
is one of their current heroes, the joke will bomb.        

I should also add that there is a difference between a hostile laugh and a sympathetic
one. What I have been analyzing refers to hostile laughter – which is by far the most
satisfying type. A sympathetic laugh is usually one which is produced in order to be
agreeable in a social situation. But sympathetic humor, such as
America’s Funniest
Home Videos
, or some kind of Animal Antics, is likely to produce more smiles than
belly laughs.

I could go on. But for an analysis of different types of comedic plays and films, go to my
website: www.miltontimmons.com and read the excerpts from my textbook,
Orientation
to Cinema
. There you will find the differences in writing and production between High
Comedy
, Low Comedy, Parody, Satire, High Camp, and Low Camp.

--Milt
Elements of Humor
[This essay was posted on the website of the Philosophy Club,
which meets regularly in Santa Monica, CA. It was an amplification
of remarks I made during the previous meeting, which was about
the philosophy of humor]
Home
Other Works Site Map